Predictores parentales de la flexibilidad cognitiva y la inhibición en escolares
Parental predictors of cognitive flexibility and inhibition in schoolchildren
Francisca Bernal-Ruiz , Bruno Falcón-Larach , Matías Ortiz , José Orellana , Diego Espinoza , Fernanda Izzo
Suma Psicológica, (2025), 32(1), pp. 76-84.
Recibido el 7 de enero de 2025
Aceptado el 29 de mayo 2025
Introducción: diversos estudios han demostrado que una parentalidad caracterizada por la sensibilidad, protección, apego y afectividad contribuye no solo a los procesos socioemocionales y de aprendizaje, sino también al desarrollo de las funciones ejecutivas en la infancia. A pesar de esta evidencia, hay inconsistencias acerca del efecto de las prácticas de crianza específicas sobre el desarrollo de estas habilidades cognitivas superiores en los niños y las niñas. Este estudio tiene como objetivo determinar la capacidad predictiva de las competencias parentales vinculares, formativas, protectoras y reflexivas en el desarrollo de las funciones ejecutivas de inhibición cognitiva y conductual y flexibilidad cognitiva en niños y niñas de entre 6 y 8 años. Método: se evaluaron 91 díadas de padres/madres/cuidadores y sus hijos/as, utilizando una escala de parentalidad positiva para los adultos y tres tareas ejecutivas para los niños y las niñas. Se realizaron análisis descriptivos, correlaciones y modelos de regresión. Resultados: indican que las competencias parentales de tipo vincular y formativo explican el desempeño de la inhibición cognitiva y conductual, pero no tienen influencia significativa en la flexibilidad cognitiva. Conclusiones: estos resultados enfatizan el rol que desempeñan las prácticas de crianza en el desarrollo cognitivo de los/as hijos/as, y aportan información relevante sobre la importancia de potenciar las competencias parentales asociadas a la crianza positiva.
Palabras clave:
Competencias parentales, crianza, funciones ejecutivas, flexibilidad cognitiva, inhibición
Introduction: Several studies have shown that parenting characterized by sensitivity, protection, attachment and affectivity contributes not only to socioemotional and learning processes, but also to the development of executive functions in childhood. Despite this evidence, there are inconsistencies about the effect of specific parental skills on the development of these higher cognitive skills in children. The aim of this study was to determine the predictive capacity of relational, formative, protective and reflective parenting skills on the development of the executive functions of cognitive and behavioral inhibition and cognitive flexibility in children aged 6 to 8 years. Method: 91 dyads of parents/caregivers and their children were evaluated, using a positive parenting scale for adults and three executive tasks for children. Descriptive analyses, correlations and regression models were performed. Results: The results indicate that relational and formative parental skills explain cognitive and behavioral inhibition performance but have no significant influence on cognitive flexibility. Conclusions: These results emphasize the role of parenting practices in the cognitive development of children, providing relevant information on the importance of enhancing parental skills associated with positive parenting.
Keywords:
Parental competencies, parenting, executive functions, cognitive flexibility, inhibition
Albert, W., Hanson, J., Skinner, A., Dodge, K., Steinberg, L., Deater-Deckard, K., Bornstein, M., & Lansford, J. (2020). Individual differences in executive function partially explain the socioeconomic gradient in middle-school academic achievement. Developmental Science, 23(5), Article e12937. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12937
American Psychological Association. (2017). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. https://www.apa.org/ethics/code
Amicarelli, A. R., Kotelnikova, Y., Smith, H. J., Kryski, K. R., & Hayden, E. P. (2018). Parenting differentially influences the development of boys’ and girls’ inhibitory control. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(3), 371-383. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12220
Anvari, F., Efendić, E., Olsen, J., Arslan, R., Elson, M., & Schneider, I. (2022). Bias in self-reports: An initial elevation phenomenon. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 14(6), 727-737. https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506221129160
Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. (2019). Neuropsicología clínica. Manual Moderno.
Ato, M., López, J., & Benavente, A. (2013). Un sistema de clasificación de los diseños de investigación en psicología. Anales de Psicología, 29(3), 1038-1059. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.29.3.178511
Aydmune, Y., Introzzi, I., Richard’s, M., Stelzer, F., & Krzemien, D. (2019). Flexibilidad cognitiva y tres procesos inhibitorios durante los primeros años de la escolaridad primaria. Subjetividad y Procesos Cognitivos, 23(2), 186-202.
Bausela, E. (2024). Comparative study of the development of executive functions in children: Transition from the first cycle to the second cycle of early childhood education. Brain Sciences, 14(12), Article 1273. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14121273
Bernal-Ruiz, F., Cucumides, P., Mauna, F., Candia, B., Díaz de Alda, M., Casas-Cordero, B., & Aguilera, C. (2023). Competencias parentales y su influencia en la inhibición y planificación de escolares. Revista ProPulsión, 6(2), 8-24. https://doi.org/10.53645/revprop.v6i1.104
Bernal-Ruiz, F., Ortega, A., & Rodríguez-Vera, M. (2020). The influence of parental skills on children executive performance in the Chilean context. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 29(11), 3103-3116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-020-01827-w
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641-1660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
Bibi, A., Hayat, R., Hayat, N., Zulfiqar, S., Shafique, N., & Khalid, M. (2022). Impact of parenting styles on psychological flexibility among adolescents of Pakistan: A cross-sectional study. Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal, 39(3), 313-322. https://doi.org./10.1007/s10560-021-00754-z
Buttelmann, F., & Karbach, J. (2017). Development and plasticity of cognitive flexibility in early and middle childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1-6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01040
Choenni, V., Lambregtse-van den Berg, M., Verhulst, F., Tiemeier, H., & Kok, R. (2019). The longitudinal relation between observed maternal parenting in the preschool period and the occurrence of child ADHD symptoms in middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(5), 755-764. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0492-9
Clark, C. A., & Woodward, L. J. (2015). Relation of perinatal risk and early parenting to executive control at the transition to school. Developmental Science, 18(4), 525-542. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12232
Conejero, Á., & Rueda, M. (2023). The interplay between socioeconomic status, parenting and temperament predicts inhibitory control at two years of age. Children, 10(6), Article 1085. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10061085
Cumming, M., Poling, D., Patwardhan, I., & Ozenbaugh, I. (2022). Executive function in kindergarten and development of behavioral problems: Moderating role of positive parenting practices. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 60, 161-172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2022.01.008
D’Affonseca, S., & de Albuquerque, L. (2013). Metaparentagem: Uma nova possibilidade de avaliar a parentagem. Psicologia em Estudo, 18(1), 83-92. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-73722013000100009
Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037-2078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006
De Beer, L., van der Vaart, L., & Uziel, L. (2024). Should positive psychology researchers control for response style? International Journal of Applied Positive Psychology, 9, 1225-1236. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41042-024-00150-9
Diamond, A. (2020). Executive functions. In A. Gallagher, C. Bulteau, D. Cohen, & J. L. Michaud (Eds.), Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 173, 225-240. Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-64150-2.00020-4
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 942-956. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620904771
Dong, S., Wang, Z., & Cheng, N. (2023). Maternal cognitive mind-mindedness and maternal time as predictors of inhibitory control in toddlerhood. Early Child Development and Care, 193(9-10), 1112-1126. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2023.2231659
Feola, B., Sand, L., Atkins, S., Bunting, M., Dougherty, M., & Bolger, D. (2023). Overlapping and unique brain responses to cognitive and response inhibition. Brain and Cognition, 166, Article 105958. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2023.105958
Flores, J., Castillo-Preciado, R., & Jiménez-Miramonte, N. (2014). Desarrollo de funciones ejecutivas, de la niñez a la juventud. Anales de Psicología, 30(2), 463-473. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.30.2.155471
Friedman, N., & Robbins, T. W. (2022). The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology, 47, 72-89. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01132-0
Geeraerts, S., Endendijk, J., Deković, M., Huijding, J., Deater-Deckard, K., & Mesman, J. (2021). Inhibitory control across the preschool years: Developmental changes and associations with parenting. Child Development, 92(1), 335-350. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13426
Gómez, E., & Contreras, L. (2019). Manual de la Escala de Parentalidad Positiva, E2P V.2. Ediciones Fundación América por la Infancia.
Kang, J., & Rodríguez, C. (2023). Spanking and executive functioning in US children: A longitudinal analysis on a matched sample. Child Abuse & Neglect, 146, Article 106474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2023.106474
Kloo, D., & Sodian, B. (2017). The developmental stability of inhibition from 2 to 5 years. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 582-595. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12197
Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. (2017). Fundamentos de neuropsicología humana. Editorial Médica Panamericana.
Lunkenheimer, E., Sturge-Apple, M., & Kelm, M. (2023). The importance of parent self-regulation and parent-child coregulation in research on parental discipline. Child Development Perspectives, 17(1), 25-31. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12470
Mueller, I., Spinath, F., Friese, M., & Hahn, E. (2023). Genetics, parenting, and family functioning-What drives the development of self-control from adolescence to adulthood? Journal of Personality, 91(2), 332-353. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12723
Nikstat, A., Beam, C., & Riemann, R. (2023). Gene-environment interplay in internalizing problem behavior. Developmental Psychology, 59(8), 1470-1483. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001567
Pećnik, N., Nieterau, M., Špaček, M., & Leijten, P. (2024). Benefits of the growing up together online parenting program for families in social services in Croatia. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 33, 554-567. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-023-02770-2
Purpura, D. J., Schmitt, S., & Ganley, C. (2017). Foundations of mathematics and literacy: The role of executive functioning components. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 153, 15-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.010
Ramírez, C. (2023). Análisis de la evolución de las funciones ejecutivas en escolares colombianos según madurez evolutiva. Panorama, 17(32), 227-242. https://doi.org/10.15765/pnrm.v17i32.3696
Rosas, R., Espinoza, V., Martínez, C., & Santa-Cruz, C. (2022). Playful testing of executive functions with Yellow-Red: Tablet-based battery for children between 6 and 11. Journal of Intelligence, 10(4), Article 125. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040125
Salehinejad, M., Ghanavati, E., Rashid, M., & Nitsche, M. (2021). Hot and cold executive functions in the brain: A prefrontal-cingular network. Brain and Neuroscience Advances, 5, 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1177/23982128211007769
Semenov, A., & Zelazo, P. (2019). Mindful family routines and the cultivation of executive function skills in childhood. Human Development, 63(2), 112-131. https://doi.org/10.1159/000503822
Skowron, E., Nekkanti, A., Skoranski, A., Scholtes, C., Lyons, E., Mills, K., Bard, D., Rock, A., Berkman, E., Bard, E., & Funderburk, B. (2024). Randomized trial of parent-child interaction therapy improves child-welfare parents’ behavior, self-regulation, and self-perceptions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 92(2), 75-92. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000859
Spiegel, J. A., Goodrich, J., Morris, B., Osborne, C., & Lonigan, C. (2021). Relations between executive functions and academic outcomes in elementary school children: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 147(4), 329-351. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000322
Taylor, Z., Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. (2015). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, effortful control, and parenting as predictors of children’s sympathy across early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 17-25. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038189
The Jamovi Project. (2021). Jamovi (versión 2.3.28) [software]. JAMOVI. https://www.jamovi.org
Valcan, D., Davis, H., & Pino-Pasternak, D. (2017). Parental behaviours predicting early childhood executive functions: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 30(3), 607-649. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-017-9411-9
Vanhala, A., Lee, K., Korhonen, J., & Aunio, P. (2023). Dimensionality of executive functions and processing speed in preschoolers. Learning and Individual Differences, 107, Article 102361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2023.102361
Verhagen, M., Derks, M., Roelofs, K., & Maciejewski, D. (2023). Behavioral inhibition, negative parenting, and social withdrawal: Longitudinal associations with loneliness during early, middle, and late adolescence. Child Development, 94(2), 512-528. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13874
Vrantsidis, D., Clark, C., Chevalier, N., Espy, K., & Wiebe, S. (2020). Socioeconomic status and executive function in early childhood: Exploring proximal mechanisms. Developmental science, 23(3), Article e12917. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12917
Wade, M., Parker, V., Tang, A., Fox, N., Zeanah, C., & Nelson, C. (2024). Linking caregiving quality during infancy to brain activity in early childhood and later executive function. Developmental Science, 27(6), Article e13517. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13517
Wang, S., & Xiaosong, G. (2024). Bidirectional relationship between positive parenting behavior and children’s self-regulation: A three-wave longitudinal study. Behavioral Sciences, 14(1), Article 38. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14010038
Werchan, D., Ku, S., Berry, D., & Blair, C. (2023). Sensitive caregiving and reward responsivity: A novel mechanism linking parenting and executive functions development in early childhood. Developmental Science, 26(2), Article e13293. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13293
Woźniak-Prus, M., Gambin, M., Sękowski, M., Cudo, A., Pisula, E., Kiepura-Nawrocka, E., Boruszak-Kiziukiewicz, J., & Kmita, G. (2024). Positive experiences in the parent-child relationship during the COVID-19 lockdown in Poland: The role of emotion regulation, empathy, parenting self-efficacy, and social support. Family Process, 63(1), 443-468. https://doi.org/10.1111/famp.12856
Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2020). The neurodevelopment of executive function skills: Implications for academic achievement gaps. Psychology & Neuroscience, 13(3), 273-298. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000208